Listening Is More Effortful For New Hearing Aid Wearers

Numerous studies have illustrated the relationship between working memory, cognitive resources and speech perception and suggest that listeners with limited working memory or cognitive resources are more likely to struggle with speech recognition in noise (Gatehouse, et al., 2003; Lunner, 2003). Conversely, larger working memory capacity may allow more rapid and successful matching between speech inputs and stored lexical templates.  This concept is described by the Ease of Language Understanding (ELU) model, which proposes that cognitive processing demands vary according to the degradation of the speech signal in different environments (Ronnberg, 2003; Ronnberg et al., 2008). In quiet, favorable listening conditions, speech inputs are easily matched to stored representations and the processing is automatic. In difficult listening environments, more explicit processing is required to match inputs to stored representations. How efficiently this goal is achieved is dependent upon working memory capacity.
Using these concepts as underpinning, Ng and her colleagues proposed that working memory and cognitive processing may have more of an impact on speech recognition for new hearing aid (سمعک) users than for experienced hearing aid (سمعک) users. Hearing aids (سمعک ها) improve speech audibility and directional microphones and noise reduction can help preserve speech in adverse listening conditions, which should reduce the need for explicit working memory processing. However, if phonological representations stored in memory have been degraded by hearing loss over time, amplified speech perceived by new hearing aid (سمعک) users will not match their stored templates. Therefore, more explicit processing in working memory may be required to identify words. Over time, as the individual becomes acclimated to the amplified sound, stored templates may adapt and become more similar to their acoustic counterparts, reducing the working memory and cognitive load requirements for correct identification. Following this reasoning, Ng and her colleagues proposed that there would be a significant relationship between cognitive functioning and speech recognition in new, first-time hearing aid (سمعک) users but that the relationship would become weaker over time as stored speech representations based on amplified sound become more established.
To examine this hypothesis, 27 first-time hearing aid (سمعک) users were recruited from a pool of subjects at a Swedish university Audiology clinic. All had mild to moderately-severe sensorineural hearing loss and no previous experience with hearing aids (سمعک ها). Nine of the subjects were fitted monaurally and 18 were fitted binaurally. Four participants had in-the-ear or canal instruments and 23 had behind-the-ear instruments. Most of the subjects became full-time hearing aid (سمعک) users and the rest were consistent, part-time users.
Approximately four months prior to being fitted with their hearing aids (سمعک ها), subjects attended an experimental session at which they completed speech recognition in noise and cognitive testing. Four cognitive tests were administered: the Reading Span test, a physical matching task, a lexical decision making task and a rhyme judgment test. The Swedish version of the Reading Span test was used to assess working memory or listeners’ ability to process and store verbal information in a parallel task design (Ronnberg et al., 1989). After hearing a list of sentences, subjects were asked to recall either the first or final word of each sentence in the list.  The test was scored according to the total number of words correctly recalled. The physical matching test (Posner & Mitchell, 1967), which measured general processing speed, required participants to judge whether two examples of the same letter were visibly identical or different in physical shape (e.g., A-A vs. A-a). Scores were based on reaction time for correct trials. The lexical decision making task required subjects to judge whether a string of 3 letters presented on a screen was a real Swedish word. Scores were based on reaction time for correct trials. The rhyme judgment test required subjects to determine whether two words, presented on a screen, rhymed or not (Baddeley & Wilson, 1985). This test was intended to measure the quality of stored phonological representations and was scored based on percentage of correct judgments.
The speech recognition in noise test was conducted again at the hearing aid (سمعک) fitting appointment (0 months) and again at approximately 3 month intervals (3 months and 6 months). The investigators chose to evaluate speech recognition and its relationship to cognitive tests at these intervals based on previous reports suggesting that a familiarization period of 4-9 weeks was required to reduce cognitive load (Rudner et al., 2011).
The results of the speech recognition in noise test showed, not surprisingly, that aided SRT was significantly better than unaided SRT.  The change in SRT over time was also significant, in that the SRT measured at 6 months was significantly better than at 0 months. The 3 month SRT was not significantly different from the 0 month or 6 month tests.  Age and pure-tone-average (PTA) were significantly correlated with SRT at 0, 3 and 6 month tests. At 0 and 3 months, the cognitive measures of reading span, physical matching and lexical decision were all correlated with SRT. At 6 months, only the correlations between lexical decision and reading span were significant. These results indicate that the relationship between cognitive measures and speech recognition declined over the first 6 months of hearing aid (سمعک) use.  Regression analysis showed a similar pattern in that reading span and PTA were significant predictors of speech recognition at 0 months, but by 6 months, only PTA was a significant predictor.
The pattern of results in this study supports the authors’ proposal that for first-time hearing aid (سمعک) users, working memory and cognitive processing play a more important role in speech perception in noise immediately after fitting than they do after acclimatization. Their hypothesis that stored perceptual representations are altered by long-term hearing loss and are therefore mismatched with newly amplified speech inputs is supported by their clinical observations. First-time hearing aid (سمعک) users typically experience amplified speech as “tinny”, “metallic”, or in some way “artificial”.  For most new hearing aid (سمعک) users, this perception resolves within a few weeks or so, though others may require longer periods of use to become acclimated. As time goes on, new hearing aid (سمعک) users usually report that speech sounds more natural and the data presented here support the assertion that stored lexical representations, after becoming distorted from long-term hearing impairment, may be adapting based on consistently restored audibility of speech sounds.
The results of this study support the importance of cognitive functioning for speech perception in noise and suggest that new hearing aid (سمعک) users experience increased cognitive demands for understanding speech as compared to experienced hearing aid (سمعک) users. It follows that individuals who have limited working memory or impaired cognition may also experience longer acclimatization periods with their new hearing aids (سمعک ها).
Clinicians are accustomed to counseling patients to wear their hearing aids (سمعک ها) consistently; for most of the day, every day.  The authors of this study did not examine the usage patterns of their subjects with reference to their hypothesis, but future studies should investigate the potential effects of limited hearing aid (سمعک) use on the relationship between cognition and speech recognition in noise. If full-time use results in a more rapidly waning relationship between these variables (indicating a more rapid decrease in cognitive load required for speech recognition) it would underscore the importance of consistent hearing aid (سمعک)use for new users, especially those with cognitive or working memory limitations.
 

 

References :Starkey Research and Clinical Blog